Gulp: Adventures on the Alimentary Canal

There is no way to know or imagine what the taste of pyrophosphate is like for cats. It’s like a cat trying to imagine the taste of sugar. Cats, unlike dogs and other omnivores, can’t taste sweetness. There’s no need, since the cat’s diet in the wild contains almost nothing in the way of carbohydrates (which include simple sugars). Either cats never had the gene for detecting sweet, or they lost it somewhere down the evolutionary road.

Rodents, on the other hand, are slaves to sweetness. They have been known to die of malnutrition rather than step away from a sugar-water drip. In an obesity study from the 1970s, rats fed an all-you-can-eat “supermarket” diet that included marshmallows, milk chocolate, and chocolate-chip cookies gained 269 percent more weight than rats fed standard laboratory fare. There are strains of mice that will, over the course of a day, consume their own bodyweight in diet soda, and you do not want the job of changing their bedding.

Does that mean rodents feel pleasure in tasting sweet things the same way we do? Or is it simply a sequence of programmed responses, receptors sending signals and signals driving muscles? Video footage Danielle Reed sent me suggests that rodents do consciously perceive and savor the taste of something sweet. One clip shows a white mouse that has just been drinking sugar solution. She is shown in ultra-slow motion, filmed from below through a clear plastic floor, licking the fur around the sides of her mouth. (The caption uses the scientific term for lip-licking: “lateral tongue protrusion.”) Another clip shows a mouse that has just tasted denatonium benzoate, a bitter compound that parents used to paint on their children’s fingertips to discourage nail-biting. The mouse is doing everything it can to rid itself of traces of the chemical. It shakes its head and rubs its face with its hairy white forelegs. It pulls a “gape”: mouth opened wide, tongue stuck out to eject the offending food. (Humans do this too. The scientific term here is “the disgust face.”)

“If it’s exceedingly nasty,” Reed told me, “they will actually drag their tongue on the bedding to try to get it off.” Clearly taste matters to them.

Conversely, do animals with no taste buds derive no pleasure from eating? Is it just a daily chore? Has anyone observed—in, say, a python eating a rat—those same parts of the brain that light up when humans are experiencing taste delight? Reed doesn’t know. “But no doubt somewhere in the world there’s a scientist trying to get a live python into an fMRI machine.”

Rawson points out that although snakes can’t taste, they have a primitive sense of smell. They’ll extend their tongue to gather volatile molecules and then pull it back in and plug it into the vomeronasal organ at the roof of the mouth to get a reading. Snakes are keenly attuned to the aroma of favored prey—so much so that if you slip a rat’s face and hide, Hannibal Lecter–style, over the snout of a non-favored prey item, a python will try to swallow it. (University of Alabama snake digestion expert Stephen Secor did this some years back to reenact a scene for National Geographic television. “Worked like a charm,” he told me. “I can get a python to eat a beer bottle if I put a rat head on it.”)


For part of their development, human fetuses have a vomeronasal organ, though no one knows whether it’s functional. You can no more ask a fetus about these things than a python. Rawson surmises that the organ is a holdover from “when we were crawling out of the primordial soup,* and we needed to sense the chemicals in the environment and know which ones to go toward or away from.”

Rawson has an idea of what it is like to eat without perceiving tastes, because she has talked to cancer patients whose taste receptors have been destroyed by radiation treatments. The situation is well beyond unpleasant. “Your body is saying, ‘It’s not food, it’s cardboard,’ and it won’t let you swallow. No matter how much you tell your brain that you need to eat to survive, you’ll gag. These people can actually die of starvation.” Rawson knows a researcher who has been experimenting with using potent flavors—which, as we know from the last chapter, are mainly smells—to make up for absent tastes. Taste and smell are intertwined in ways we don’t consciously appreciate. Food technologists sometimes exploit the synergy between the two. By adding strawberry or vanilla—aromas we associate with sweetness—it’s possible to fool people into thinking a food is sweeter than it really is. Though sneaky, this is not necessarily bad, because it means the product can contain less added sugar.

Mary Roach's books