Everybody Lies: Big Data, New Data, and What the Internet Can Tell Us About Who We Really Are

The answer: students, like Yilmaz, who scored very, very close to the cutoff necessary to attend Stuyvesant.* Students who just missed the cutoff are the control group; students who just made the cut are the treatment group.

There is little reason to suspect students on either side of the cutoff differ much in talent or drive. What, after all, causes a person to score just a point or two higher on a test than another? Maybe the lower-scoring one slept ten minutes too little or ate a less nutritious breakfast. Maybe the higher-scoring one had remembered a particularly difficult word on the test from a conversation she had with her grandmother three years earlier.

In fact, this category of natural experiments—utilizing sharp numerical cutoffs—is so powerful that it has its own name among economists: regression discontinuity. Anytime there is a precise number that divides people into two different groups—a discontinuity—economists can compare—or regress—the outcomes of people very, very close to the cutoff.

Two economists, M. Keith Chen and Jesse Shapiro, took advantage of a sharp cutoff used by federal prisons to test the effects of rough prison conditions on future crime. Federal inmates in the United States are given a score, based on the nature of their crime and their criminal history. The score determines the conditions of their prison stay. Those with a high enough score will go to a high-security correctional facility, which means less contact with other people, less freedom of movement, and likely more violence from guards or other inmates.

Again, it would not be fair to compare the entire universe of prisoners who went to high-security prisons to the entire universe of prisoners who went to low-security prisons. High-security prisons will include more murderers and rapists, low-security prisons more drug offenders and petty thieves.

But those right above or right below the sharp numerical threshold had virtually identical criminal histories and backgrounds. This one measly point, however, meant a very different prison experience.

The result? The economists found that prisoners assigned to harsher conditions were more likely to commit additional crimes once they left. The tough prison conditions, rather than deterring them from crime, hardened them and made them more violent once they returned to the outside world.

So what did such a “regression discontinuity” show for Stuyvesant High School? A team of economists from MIT and Duke—Atila Abdulkadirog?lu, Joshua Angrist, and Parag Pathak—performed the study. They compared the outcomes of New York pupils on both sides of the cutoff. In other words, these economists looked at hundreds of students who, like Yilmaz, missed Stuyvesant by a question or two. They compared them to hundreds of students who had a better test day and made Stuy by a question or two. Their measures of success were AP scores, SAT scores, and the rankings of the colleges they eventually attended.

Their stunning results were made clear by the title they gave the paper: “Elite Illusion.” The effects of Stuyvesant High School? Nil. Nada. Zero. Bupkus. Students on either side of the cutoff ended up with indistinguishable AP scores and indistinguishable SAT scores and attended indistinguishably prestigious universities.

The entire reason that Stuy students achieve more in life than non-Stuy students, the researchers concluded, is that better students attend Stuyvesant in the first place. Stuy does not cause you to perform better on AP tests, do better on your SATs, or end up at a better college.

“The intense competition for exam school seats,” the economists wrote, “does not appear to be justified by improved learning for a broad set of students.”

Why might it not matter which school you go to? Some more stories can help get at the answer. Consider two more students, Sarah Kaufmann and Jessica Eng, two young New Yorkers who both dreamed from an early age of going to Stuy. Kaufmann’s score was just on the cutoff; she made it by one question. “I don’t think anything could be that exciting again,” Kaufmann recalls. Eng’s score was just below the cutoff; she missed by one question. Kaufmann went to her dream school, Stuy. Eng did not.

So how did their lives end up? Both have since had successful, and rewarding, careers—as do most people who score in the top 5 percent of all New Yorkers on tests. Eng, ironically, enjoyed her high school experience more. Bronx Science, where she attended, was the only high school with a Holocaust museum. Eng discovered she loved curation and studied anthropology at Cornell.

Kaufmann felt a little lost in Stuy, where students were heavily focused on grades and she felt there was too much emphasis on testing, not on teaching. She called her experience “definitely a mixed bag.” But it was a learning experience. She realized, for college, she would only apply to liberal arts schools, which had more emphasis on teaching. She got accepted to her dream school, Wesleyan University. There she found a passion for helping others, and she is now a public interest lawyer.

People adapt to their experience, and people who are going to be successful find advantages in any situation. The factors that make you successful are your talent and your drive. They are not who gives your commencement speech or other advantages that the biggest name-brand schools offer.

This is only one study, and it is probably weakened by the fact that most of the students who just missed the Stuyvesant cutoff ended up at another fine school. But there is growing evidence that, while going to a good school is important, there is little gained from going to the greatest possible school.

Take college. Does it matter if you go to one of the best universities in the world, such as Harvard, or a solid school such as Penn State?

Once again, there is a clear correlation between the ranking of one’s school and how much money people make. Ten years into their careers, the average graduate of Harvard makes $123,000. The average graduate of Penn State makes $87,800.

But this correlation does not imply causation.

Two economists, Stacy Dale and Alan B. Krueger, thought of an ingenious way to test the causal role of elite universities on the future earning potential of their graduates. They had a large dataset that tracked a whole host of information on high school students, including where they applied to college, where they were accepted to college, where they attended college, their family background, and their income as adults.

Seth Stephens-Davidowitz's books